5 Ways the GOP Can Win Votes—No Candidates Necessary!
Psychological priming can impact voters’ choices.
The GOP is in trouble. Republican presidential candidate Donald Trump lags behind the Democratic challenger Hillary Clinton in the national polls. Which polls, you may ask? All of them. Even more troubling for those on the political right, a landslide for Clinton in November could shift the political landscape well beyond the White House. Concern is growing in the GOP that down-ballot Republicans—from senators and representatives in Congress all the way down to local dogcatchers—might be swept aside if voters favoring Clinton turn out en masse and vote straight ticket. Can Republicans hold the Senate? What about the House? Can anything be done? Well, one option is to push ahead while ignoring the candidates. All of them.
In recent years, in both controlled experiments and by examining troves of detailed election data, psychologists, behavioral economists and other scholars have made important discoveries about what sways voters to pick one candidate or party over another. Some of this research has looked past the candidates themselves, instead examining factors that might at first seem irrelevant, but upon closer inspection appear to correlate with or even cause a voting preference. The results, to say the least, have been rather unexpected. For example, a study of UK polling data newly published in the British Journal of Political Science found that for the Tories, size matters: “taller individuals are more likely to support the Conservative Party, support conservative policies and vote Conservative,” the researchers noted, adding that “a one-inch increase in height increases support for Conservatives by 0.6 per cent.”
Elongating the electorate isn’t exactly a workable strategy. Instead, what should interest right-of-center politicians on this side of the Atlantic are the discoveries associated with subliminal persuasion. Priming, as explained by Cass Sunstein and Richard Thaler in their popular 2008 book Nudge, “refers to the somewhat mysterious workings of the Automatic System of the brain. Research shows that subtle influences can increase the ease with which certain information comes to mind. . . . Sometimes the merest hint of an idea or concept will trigger an association that can stimulate action. These ‘primes’ occur in social situations, and their effects can be surprisingly powerful.” In other words, people, places and things, even if they are in the background, have a remarkable and often unconscious impact on choice.
Another scholar who has contributed important findings in this field is the Nobel laureate Daniel Kahneman. He described several examples of priming in his 2011 book Thinking, Fast and Slow. In one such study of voting on school funding, for example, researchers found that support for more funds was higher in polling stations that were located inside schools. Location mattered. Being at the site of what the money would support influenced some voters whether they realized it or not. And there’s more: ideas alone can prime voters. Another study found that support for more school funding was also higher among voters who were simply shown pictures of classrooms and lockers ahead of voting. The participants did not need to be physically standing in the classroom for the effect to take hold; the images were enough. Nor was the impact trivial. “The effect of the images was larger than the difference between parents and other voters!” Kahneman noted.
Priming has been shown in studies to influence everything from locomotion speed to spending habits—and, yes, voting preference. What has been discovered so far—and the field is still relatively new—has been surprising and thought-provoking. It has also been unsettling to the idea that people are the commanders-in-chief of their own choices. As Kahneman explained, “Studies of priming effects have yielded discoveries that threaten our self-image as conscious and autonomous authors of our judgments and our choices. . . . We now know the effects of priming can reach into every corner of our lives.” Moreover, recent research into similar forms of “nudging” has shown that these cues are effective even when rendered transparent, even when individuals know they are being purposefully influenced.
So, culling through the available research, here are five ways Republicans might want to consider priming voters ahead of election day.
1. Fifty Stars and Thirteen Stripes
In 2011, three scholars from the University of Chicago, Cornell University and Hebrew University published a scorcher of a research paper in the journal Psychological Science. People’s political judgments, the researchers claimed, could be influenced toward supporting Republican causes by an item readily available in any local Wal-Mart: the American flag. In a set of controlled experiments conducted between 2008 and 2010, the researchers exposed some participants to images of The Stars and Stripes—for example, a 72-by-45 pixel flag in the corner of the computer screen while taking a survey. When participants were then given a subsequent survey to measure attitudes toward political issues, the results revealed that the flag-primed participants skewed markedly rightward over a control group. There was “a significant increase in participants’ Republican voting intentions, voting behavior, political beliefs, and implicit and explicit attitudes.” Exposure to the American flag, the researchers concluded, “significantly shifted both Democratic and Republican participants’ beliefs, attitudes, and voting behavior toward Republicanism.” What’s more, some of the shifts seemed to stick around for up to eight months after the initial priming. “This prolonged influence represents one of the most durable priming effects in the cognitive sciences literature,” they wrote.
Here, it seems, is a remedy for Republican woes: line the streets with American flags and Old Glory will raise support for the Grand Old Party. Alas, it’s not so simple. A different set of researchers then tried to replicate the astonishing results, publishing their work in 2014 in the journal Social Psychology. It didn’t go as expected. They failed to get the same results.
The original three researchers, however, were unwilling to declare a total defeat. They responded that “[a] different political atmosphere, different subject pools, and different states of mind separate the original and the replication attempt. For these reasons, we view this as a conceptual, and not a direct, replication.” Facts had changed, they argued. Moreover, since the original study was published additional work had been ongoing to identify the precise conditions in which people are primed by the flag, to what extent and for how long. The Star Spangled Banner thesis may yet survive in some form.
“Since the Civil War, Americans have been a flag-oriented people,” Samuel Huntington wrote. “The Stars and Stripes has the status of a religious icon and is a more central symbol of national identity for Americans than their flags are for peoples of other nations.” All true, but more to the point, flags are low cost and abundant. The United States imported $4.4 million worth of American flags last year alone! Lining the streets in red, white and blue could be accomplished relatively easily. If there’s even a minute, highly qualified chance flags might work as a prime that shifts voters rightward, Republicans might consider running this idea up the flagpole.
2. Keep the End in Mind
The Republican National Convention in Cleveland was a lively event. But amid the major key, allegro brillante trumpeting that characterized much of the oration in the Quicken Loans Arena there were also, to the keen listener, a few melancholy notes interspersed. On the second night, for example, businessman Andy Wist blurted out toward the end of his speech, “I’m not going to be around forever, and neither will you be.” As the Washington Post’s Alexandra Petri pithily tweeted at the time, “mortality yay.” Wist’s wistful memento mori was an odd, downbeat moment in what should have been an uplifting celebration. It was more in the mood of Hamlet, when the occasion clearly called for Henry V. Yet alas, poor Yorick, Wist might have been on to something.
In their book The Worm at the Core (reviewed in the Sept-Oct 2015 issue of the National Interest), Jeff Greenberg, Sheldon Solomon and Tom Pyszczynski described their research on how reminders of death influence decision making. In one such experiment, participants were asked to select between three hypothetical political candidates. One candidate signaled a commitment to accomplishing goals. Another emphasized shared responsibility. A final candidate was presented as “charismatic, bold, self-confident, and emphasized the group’s greatness.” A portion of these participants were reminded of death before selecting among the candidates. The results were astonishing—an eightfold increase in votes for the charismatic candidate in the group that was reminded of death over the control group. “Intimations of mortality amplify the allure of charismatic leaders,” the researchers explained.
This same result played out with flesh-and-blood candidates as well, Greenberg, Solomon and Pyszczynski noted. In an experiment held before the 2004 U.S. presidential election, participants who had been reminded of death reported by an almost 3-to-1 margin that they would vote for President George W. Bush over Senator John Kerry. At the time, the researchers explained, President Bush was seen as the “charismatic” candidate purveying the message that “we are divinely ordained to defeat the forces of evil” after the grim shock of September 11, 2001.
In all, this research suggests that—after a reminder of death, of course—voters might be more inclined to support a “charismatic, bold, self-confident” candidate. These are qualities Trump has in excess. Importantly, the candidate would also need to espouse the idea that America is “divinely ordained to defeat the forces of evil.” Perhaps it was no coincidence then that in his much-touted foreign policy speech on August 15, Trump emphasized this point about fighting terrorism: “we cannot let this evil continue.”
3. Take Me to Church
Demarcating the separation between Church and State is a solemn responsibility placed on the highest echelons of American government—the Supreme Court, for example. Yet it is the church down the street, a place much closer to home, where this boundary might be most meaningful come November 8.
In 2010, the journal Political Psychology published an important paper by Abraham Rutchick of California State University, Northridge. It was the result of an idea Rutchick wanted to test that ecclesiastical primes might influence political choice. As noted above, it has been shown that setting up polling stations in schools resulted in an increase in voters favoring school funding proposals. Similarly, Rutchick thought that holding voting in churches might prime more voters to support conservative causes and candidates. To study this idea he dove into the 2004 data from South Carolina’s sixth congressional district. In the end, the results were clear. Adjusting for precinct-level partisan composition, he found that the more conservative candidate did statistically better in voting precincts located in churches than in those located elsewhere—41 percent support in churches to 32 percent elsewhere, in this case.
Importantly, in broader follow-on studies Rutchick found that such primes only affected self-identified Christians. He explained, “ecclesiastical images have their impact by increasing Christian identity, which in turn activates Christian values and their implications for political issues.” Nonetheless, the full set of studies he performed led Rutchick to conclude that “churches as polling places could be advantageous to politically conservative candidates and to supporters of conservative positions on abortion, same-sex marriage, and other relevant issues.” These are candidates, in other words, that tend to be Republican.
4. Make It NOT Rain
A downpour on election day might result in a poor showing at the polls. After all, who wants to wait in long lines in the rain? But Republicans interested in boosting votes for their party on November 8 might instead want to worry about the weather a bit earlier in the year on July 4, America’s Independence Day. Here’s why.
In a 2012 working paper for Harvard Kennedy School, two researchers looked at Americans born between 1920 and 1990 to see if the number of rainy Independence Days during childhood influenced political choice. Their thinking was twofold. First, more rain-free Fourths might mean more patriotic festivities, more fireworks, more flags and more U.S.-themed hoopla. All of these things contribute to a sense of “national identity and a belief in the underlying principles supporting American society.” Second, more rain-free Fourths might also mean a stronger identity as Republican for all of the reasons cited in the 2011 flag-priming study. The results supported both ideas. At the sample mean age of 39, for example, the likelihood that an adult turns out to vote is increased by 0.88 percentage points for each rain-free Fourth of July in childhood. Meanwhile, the likelihood an adult identifies as Republican increases by 0.61 percentage points.
What does this mean for 2016? After all, there isn’t much that can be done about the weather on of Fourth of July now. Still, Republicans might do well to note that Arizona and Nevada contain six of the driest ten sites monitored by NOAA in the lower forty-eight states. Yuma, for example, gets barely over three inches of precipitation a year. Importantly, these states also could go either way in the election, with Trump now slightly ahead in Arizona and slightly behind in Nevada. If there is any residual priming from previous rain-free Independence Days, perhaps here is where Republicans should direct their resources. Of course, this would mean fewer resources for a state like New Hampshire, where Trump is currently getting soaked by Clinton but where Trump’s running mate nonetheless has spent time in the last week. Incidentally, New Hampshire also happens to be the home of Mount Washington, the site in the contiguous United States that sees the most precipitation each year.
5. Ask and You Shall Receive
The biggest challenge for Republicans and Democrats on election day may not be persuading whatever undecided Americans remain to favor one candidate over another. Instead, it may be getting people who already favor their side to vote at all. In other words, turnout may be the decisive factor come November 8. Here Sunstein and Thaler offer an intriguing and elegantly simple idea from their book Nudge: “It turns out that if you ask people, the day before the election, whether they intend to vote, you can increase the probability of their voting by as much as 25 percent!”
A strategy of asking already Republican-leaning voters on November 7 if they intend to vote tomorrow might yield more votes, more easily, at less cost than trying to persuade those leaning slightly left to vote Republican at the last minute instead. Moreover, Sunstein and Thaler point out that this prime can be accentuated by getting to the specifics—by asking people not just if they will vote, but when and where they plan to vote.
There is no perfect prime, no hypnotic cue that invariable persuades a person to vote one way or another. But, research is clear that subtle influences can and do nudge human thoughts and actions. If nothing else, an election strategy that is open to incorporating primes might offer a bit of solace to any Republicans currently dismayed by their presidential candidate’s faltering poll numbers, or even to those simply interested in supporting down-ballot GOP candidates. Of course, Democrats could also seek out primes to sway voters this November. But then again, their candidate is already leading in the national polls. All of them.
John Richard Cookson is a writer in Washington, DC.
Image: Sparkler before a U.S. flag. Pixabay/Public domain